Empathy and cooperation go hand in hand - Neuroscience News (2024)

Summary: A capacity for empathy is key in sustaining cooperation between a social group. Findings reveal empathy can evolve through a process of social contagion.

Source: University of Pennsylvania

It’s a big part of what makes us human: we cooperate. But humans aren’t saints. Most of us are more likely to help someone we consider good than someone we consider a jerk.

How we form these moral assessments of others has a lot to do with cultural and social norms, as well as our capacity for empathy, the extent to which we can take on the perspective of another person.

In a new analysis, researchers from the University of Pennsylvania investigate cooperation with an evolutionary approach. Using game-theory-driven models, they show that a capacity for empathy fosters cooperation, according to senior author Joshua Plotkin, an evolutionary biologist. The models also show that the extent to which empathy promotes cooperation depends on a given society’s system for moral evaluation.

“Having not just the capacity but the willingness to take into account someone else’s perspective when forming moral judgments tends to promote cooperation,” says Plotkin.

What’s more, the group’s analysis points to a heartening conclusion. All else being equal, empathy tends to spread throughout a population under most scenarios.

“We asked, ‘can empathy evolve?'” explains Arunas Radzvilavicius, the study’s lead author and a postdoctoral researcher who works with Plotkin. “What if individuals start copying the empathetic way of observing each other’s interactions? And we saw that empathy soared through the population.”

Plotkin and Radzvilavicius coauthored the study, published today in eLife, with Alexander Stewart, an assistant professor at the University of Houston.

Plenty of scientists have probed the question of why individuals cooperate through indirect reciprocity, a scenario in which one person helps another not because of a direct quid pro quo but because they know that person to be “good.” But the Penn group gave the study a nuance that others had not explored. Whereas other studies have assumed that reputations are universally known, Plotkin, Radzvilavicius, and Stewart realized this did not realistically describe human society, where individuals may differ in their opinion of others’ reputations.

“In large, modern societies, people disagree a lot about each other’s moral reputations,” Plotkin says.

The researchers incorporated this variation in opinions into their models, which imagine someone choosing either to donate or not to donate to a second person based on that individual’s reputation. The researchers found that cooperation was less likely to be sustained when people disagree about each other’s reputations.

That’s when they decided to incorporate empathy, or theory of mind, which, in the context of the study, entails the ability to understand the perspective of another person.

Doing so allowed cooperation to win out over more selfish strategies.

“It makes a lot of sense,” Plotkin says. “If I don’t account for your point of view, there will be many occasions when I judge you harshly when I really shouldn’t because, from your perspective, you were doing the right thing.”

To further explore the impact of empathy on cooperation, the researchers looked at a variety of frameworks, or social norms, that people might use to assign a reputation to another person based on their behavior. For example, most frameworks label someone “good” if they reward a fellow “good” individual, but social norms differ in how they judge interactions with a person deemed bad. While the “stern judging” norm labels “good” anyone who punishes a bad actor, the “simple standing” norm does not require this punitive approach: A “good” person can reward a bad one.

Plotkin, Radzvilavicius, and Stewart discovered again that capacity for empathy mattered. When populations were empathetic, stern judging was the best at promoting cooperation. But when a group was less willing to take on the perspective of another, other norms maximized rates of cooperation.

This result prompted the team to ask another evolutionary question–whether empathy itself can evolve and become stable in a population. And under most scenarios, the answer was yes.

“Starting with a population where no one is empathetic, with people judging each other based on their own perspective, we saw that eventually, individuals will copy the behavior of those who judge empathetically,” says Plotkin. “Empathy will spread, and cooperation can emerge.”

This was the case even when the researchers accounted for a degree of errors, noise, and misperception in their models.

The findings open up a new area of research for both evolutionary theory and empirical studies into how societies behave.

“Empathy is completely foreign to game theory,” Radzvilavicius say. “In a way this is finding a new niche for research to progress to in the future, accounting for theory of mind.”

Looking ahead, the Penn team hopes to pursue such questions, perhaps by pitting different social norms against one another and eventually by testing their ideas against observations from real people, either through experiments they design or through data collected from social media.

“It’s obvious that in social media people are acutely aware of their public persona and reputation and curate it carefully,” Plotkin says. “It would be fascinating to analyze these evolutionary dynamics as they play out in online interactions.”

Funding: The study was supported by the David and Lucile Packard Foundation and the U.S. Army Research Office (Grant W911NF-12-R-0012-04).

Joshua B. Plotkin is a professor in the Department of Biology at the University of Pennsylvania School of Arts and Sciences. He has secondary appointments in the Department of Mathematics and the School of Engineering and Applied Science’s Department of Computer and Information Science.

Arunas L. Radzvilavicius is a postdoctoral researcher in Penn’s Department of Biology.

Alexander J. Stewart is an assistant professor at the University of Houston and a former postdoctoral researcher at Penn.

About this neuroscience research article

Source:
University of Pennsylvania
Media Contacts:
Katherine Unger Baillie – University of Pennsylvania
Image Source:
The image is in the public domain.

Original Research: Closed access.
“Evolution of empathetic moral evaluation”.
Arunas L Radzvilavicius Is a corresponding author , Alexander J Stewart, Joshua B Plotkin. eLife doi:10.7554/eLife.44269

Abstract

Evolution of empathetic moral evaluation

Social norms can promote cooperation by assigning reputations to individuals based on their past actions. A good reputation indicates that an individual is likely to reciprocate. A large body of research has established norms of moral assessment that promote cooperation, assuming reputations are objective. But without a centralized institution to provide an objective evaluation, opinions about an individual’s reputation may differ across a population. In this setting, we study the role of empathy-the capacity to form moral evaluations from another person’s perspective. We show that empathy tends to foster cooperation by reducing the rate of unjustified defection. The norms of moral evaluation previously considered most socially beneficial depend on high levels of empathy, whereas different norms maximize social welfare in populations incapable of empathy. Finally, we show that empathy itself can evolve through social contagion. We conclude that a capacity for empathy is a key component for sustaining cooperation in societies.

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.

Empathy and cooperation go hand in hand - Neuroscience News (2024)

FAQs

Empathy and cooperation go hand in hand - Neuroscience News? ›

Summary: A capacity for empathy is key in sustaining cooperation between a social group. Findings reveal empathy can evolve through a process of social contagion. It's a big part of what makes us human: we cooperate.

What is the relationship between empathy and cooperation? ›

Empathy is the cornerstone of healthy relationships and the ability to navigate complex social situations. The cognitive system that produces empathy, the left ventral striatum, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, and supplemental motor area, motivates cooperation with others.

What is the neuroscience behind empathy? ›

Neuroimaging studies indicate that the same areas of the brain are activated when people experience their own emotions and when they observe such emotions in others. Sharing an emotional state with others is, thus, an important aspect of empathizing.

What is the difference between empathy and compassion in neuroscience? ›

Compassion goes beyond feeling with the other to feeling for the other. Unlike empathy, compassion increases activity in the areas of the brain involved in dopaminergic reward and oxytocin-related affiliative processes, and enhances positive emotions in response to adverse situations (8).

How can we maintain empathy and cooperation? ›

Read Other People. Another way to enhance empathy and cooperation is to focus on building your skills in reading and responding to other people. In psychology, this is referred to as Theory of Mind. Essentially, it's the ability to understand and take the perspective of another person.

What are the three sides of empathy? ›

Overview
  • 1) Emotional empathy: feeling what another person is feeling.
  • 2) Cognitive empathy: thinking about another person's situation.
  • 3) Behavioral empathy: being compassionate and taking actions to help based on your understanding of the situation.

What do neuroscientists believe are related to empathy? ›

Neuroscientists believe that the areas of the brain typically activated by our own emotions are also active when we observe another individual experiencing feelings or sensations. Evidence suggests that mirror neurons are strongly associated with human empathy.

What in the brain causes lack of empathy? ›

For example, the right supramarginal gyrus helps people overcome egocentric bias (self-centeredness) when making decisions. The orbitofrontal cortex is another area of the brain that helps us react to another person's feelings. Therefore, if any of these brain regions become damaged, a lack of empathy can occur.

What is the root cause of empathy? ›

Empathy seems to arise over time as part of human development, and it also has roots in evolution. In fact, “Elementary forms of empathy have been observed in our primate relatives, in dogs, and even in rats,” the Greater Good Science Center says.

What part of the brain controls empathy? ›

In the past decade, scientists have used powerful functional MRI imaging to identify several regions in the brain that are associated with empathy for pain. This most recent study, however, firmly establishes that the anterior insular cortex is where the feeling of empathy originates.

What is it when someone lacks empathy? ›

Two psychological terms particularly associated with a lack of empathy are sociopathy and psychopathy. Psychopathy, which comes from the Greek roots psykhe, which refers to the mind, and pathos, which means suffering, has shifted in popular meaning over the years, but it has always been associated with mind sickness.

What happens to the brain when you feel empathy? ›

Empathy (pain for another) elicits brain activity in the anterior cingulate and anterior insular cortex. However, empathy is also associated with caring for another (or loving another), and this part of empathy can be elicited by the hypothalamic neurohormone, oxytocin.

How would you describe the relationship between empathy and collaboration? ›

Effective collaboration is fueled by empathy—an awareness of others and an ability to detect their emotions and understand their perspective. To come up with truly innovative solutions requires new ideas.

How does empathy affect teamwork? ›

When managers show empathy in the workplace, they improve their effectiveness and increase trust and collaboration on their teams. Empathetic leadership is an asset to organizations because it increases performance and culture.

What is the importance of compassion and cooperation? ›

Compassion is foundational to building and maintaining healthy relationships. In compassionate communities, people are more likely to support each other through life's challenges, creating an atmosphere of trust, cooperation, and mutual respect. It is so important that we prioritise important relationships.

How empathy can enhance group collaboration? ›

Empathetic leadership enables employees to understand and appreciate the perspectives and emotions of their colleagues, leading to improved communication and collaboration. By fostering a sense of empathy, team members can work together more effectively, leading to greater efficiency and productivity.

References

Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Kimberely Baumbach CPA

Last Updated:

Views: 6314

Rating: 4 / 5 (61 voted)

Reviews: 84% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Kimberely Baumbach CPA

Birthday: 1996-01-14

Address: 8381 Boyce Course, Imeldachester, ND 74681

Phone: +3571286597580

Job: Product Banking Analyst

Hobby: Cosplaying, Inline skating, Amateur radio, Baton twirling, Mountaineering, Flying, Archery

Introduction: My name is Kimberely Baumbach CPA, I am a gorgeous, bright, charming, encouraging, zealous, lively, good person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.